Sexual behavior in adolescence can have a wide variety of consequences. In estimation, nearly half of all U.S. high school students have had sexual intercourse; the average age of first intercourse for boys and girls is 15. According to the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC), an average of 40,000-80,000 new cases of HIV are reported each year; approximately half of the new infections are among people younger than 25. On average, 12 million new cases of sexually transmitted diseases (STD’s) being reported each year and adolescents have among the highest-reported rates of gonorrhea and Chlamydia. Drawing on material from the course, respond to the following questions:
Need Help Writing an Essay?
Tell us about your assignment and we will find the best writer for your paper.
Write My Essay For Me1.) What cognitive and social factors contribute to high-risk sexual behavior in some adolescents?
2.) Should schools have sexual education programs in which they can inform and advise teens about sexual behavior, risk factors, and prevention? Why or why not?
Use the link below to help guide your discussion.
www.cdc.gov/HealthyYouth/sexualbehaviors/index.htm
READING
This lesson will explore the developmental journey of newborn infants, children and adolescents. We will look at newborn reflexes and infant states, or patterns of sleeping and waking, which includes crying. Next, we will look at infant sensory and perceptual capabilities, which include the auditory, visual, taste, smell and touch functions. We will also discuss how these capabilities are coordinated in intermodal perception. This will lead us to investigate how learning and memory develop. The lesson will then explore the development of children, and we will look at the basic functioning of the brain, motor development and physical growth. We will cover the importance of good nutrition and lifestyle habits in the context of healthy physical and psychological development. We will also investigate the important role of the family and social environment in this area. The last section of the lesson will continue focusing on the importance of family relationships, and will concentrate on adolescent development and the adjustments that teenagers grapple with in this stage of development.
Newborns
Reflexes are among the newborn’s first behaviors. All reflexes are involuntary and serve to promote survival. Some are permanent, such as blinking, while others are impermanent and are replaced by voluntary behaviors. Sucking for example, gets replaced by eating. Newborn reflexes that are too weak, too strong, absent or out of place can be used to predict visual, hearing, neurological and other abnormalities that may not present until years later.
The Brazelton Neonatal Assessment Scale is widely used to assess motor, learning and sensory capabilities. Interestingly, Nugent, Lester and Brazelton (1991) found that babies who are carried on their mothers’ backs in slings, as in African traditions, have better motor abilities because they use their muscles to grip onto the mother.
Watch this video to see how the reflexes of a newborn can be tested.
Infant States
INFANT STATES
Infant states are the patterns of sleeping and waking. While behavior does occur as a response to the environment, patterns or biorhythms indicate that behavior is also organized, predictable and governed by internal forces. Newborns nap throughout the day and night, but by about eight weeks they begin to sleep more through the night and less in the day. Infants learn to control their states of wakefulness so that by the end of the first year most infants have adapted their patterns to the external world and sleep through the night (Ikonomov, Stoynev, & Shisheva, 1998).
CRYING
Infants cry to communicate their needs to caregivers. Pediatricians use cries to identify illnesses. Three patterns of crying have been identified by (Schaffer, 1971). Basic crying is primarily linked to hunger, gets progressively louder and has a rhythmic sequence of cry, rest, inhale, rest. Angry crying may be caused when something the infant wants is removed, such as a pacifier. It is not as rhythmic as the basic cry, and crying segments are longer than the basic cry. When babies cry from pain or discomfort, it starts off loud, has long crying segments and long silences in which the infant gasps or holds its breath. A higher pitch may indicate colic, while delayed crying to pain stimulus may indicate brain damage.
A MOTHER’S INTERPRETATION OF CRIES
Most mothers know what their babies’ cries mean, and as infants get older their communication becomes less about their physical needs and more about their psychological needs (Kopp, 1994). Therefore, if a parent can accurately identify the meaning of their baby’s cries, they can respond promptly when the infant is distressed, but delay their response when the infant is fussing about a minor issue, and thereby bolster their infant’s self-sufficiency and decrease its fussiness (Park & Gauvain, 2009). However, ignoring a distressed cry can have severely negative consequences.
Infants learn how to relieve distress by soothing themselves. Sucking reduces infant stress especially if it is accompanied by eye contact from the caregiver (Zeifman, Delaney, & Blass, 1996).
SWADDLING
Swaddling involves tightly wrapping the baby in a blanket so its arms and legs are immobile. Swaddling is used in many cultures and hospitals around the world to soothe newborns (Valsiner, 1989). Massaging, rocking and particularly holding to the shoulder are effective soothing techniques
A MOTHER’S INTERPRETATION OF CRIES
SWADDLING
Infant Sensory and Perceptual Capacities
‹›
· Stimuli
Stimuli are received through sensations detected by the eyes, ears, nose, mouth and skin, while perceptions interpret the stimuli that have been detected. Infants are particularly tuned in to the social environment whereby infants respond to the familiar faces, voices and smells of caregivers, thus eliciting caregiver interest which promotes infant survival.
The auditory system is fully developed before birth, and studies have determined that babies can hear and learn in utero (DeCasper & Fifer,1980; Kisilevsky & Muir, 1991). Infants are born with good hearing and can identify familiar voices. They respond more to higher pitched and slightly louder than normal sounds, as well as to human voices over other sounds (Saffran, Werker, & Werner, 2006). Motherese is the infant-directed speech that mothers and caregivers tend to naturally adopt when speaking to infants, whereby their voice becomes louder, higher-pitched and more melodic. This forms the basis of speech patterns which are foundational for the development of language.
In the first year, auditory control fluctuates between the cortical and subcortical regions of the brain, causing auditory capacity to fluctuate. After the first year, babies can detect when a sound is approaching them (Morrongiello, Hewitt & Gotowiec 1991). Hearing difficulties are usually only detected after two and a half years of age as this is the period of language acquisition in which hearing difficulty becomes most evident. Ear infections can impair hearing, and it is best to have early and regular checkups to identify problems as early as possible.
Vision
· VISUAL ACUITY
· PATTERNS
· DEPTH PERCEPTION
· FEAR OF HEIGHTS
Visual acuity is how sharp vision is. While newborns can see at birth, their visual acuity is quite low unless objects are very close. As they grow, their visual acuity improves, and within a year it is within the same range as adults (Banks & Shannon, 1993). Newborn color vision is also limited, and by four months it reaches that of adults (Kellman & Arterberry, 2006). Studies suggest that infants may need to be exposed to colors for normal color vision to develop.
Touch, Taste, and Smell
· TOUCH, TASTE, AND SMELL
· TOUCH
· COORDINATING INFORMATION
Touch, taste and smell are also well developed in newborns. Their facial expressions show distaste to foul odors such as rotten eggs, and pleasure towards pleasant odors like fruit (Steiner, 1979). They have similar reactions with pleasant and unpleasant food tastes (Rosenstein & Oster, 1988). The fetus learns about taste and develops taste preferences in utero, and newborns develop preferences for tastes they are familiar with. Therefore, the more varied the mother’s diet, especially if breastfeeding, the more open to varied tastes the child will be (leCanuet, Fifer, Krasnegor, & Smotherman, 1995; Mennella & Beauchamp 1996. This also means that pregnant and breastfeeding mothers should avoid eating sugary and other unhealthy foods to minimize the risk that their babies will learn to prefer these unhealthy foods.
Watch this video on the visual cliff experiment.
Early Learning and Memory
We have been looking at how infants learn by encountering and interacting with properties through their senses. Recall that in the first lesson, we discussed learning theories, and specifically classical and operant conditioning. We will now look at how babies learn through observation, association and imitation, and how babies are able to process different kinds of information with greater efficiency as they develop.
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING
OPERANT CONDITIONING
IMITATION
MEMORY
Knowledge Check
1
Question 1
How can caregivers enhance the development of newborns?
Provide colorful surroundings, and allow the infant to explore objects, distances and movement.
Ignore their crying because they should learn early on that people will not always be there for them.
Pregnant and breastfeeding women should stick to a limited diet.
By talking to them in a mature, matter-of-fact manner.
I don’t know
One attempt
Submit answer
You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.
Brain Development
While children’s growth rates vary considerably, girls generally develop faster than boys. We will now explore physical development from infancy to childhood.
The cerebrum is the largest part of our brain. It comprises two hemispheres, and controls speech, self-awareness, sensory perception, memory and motor abilities. The cerebral cortex covers the cerebrum, and is connected to thought, speech, emotion, movement, sight and hearing.
Brain growth occurs most rapidly from the prenatal period until the age of three. The brain only stops growing in early adulthood, but studies indicate that adult brains continue to regenerate nerve cells (Gould, Reeves, Graziano, & Gross, 1999; Rosenzweig, Leiman & Breedlove, 1996). Brain plasticity refers to how the brain adapts to the environment, even in adulthood.
CHECK OUT THIS FASCINATING VIDEO ON NEURAL PLASTICITY.
NEURONS
SYNAPSES
NEURAL MIGRATION
IMPORTANCE OF SYNAPSES
REFLEXIVE AND VOLUNTARY BEHAVIOR
CORPUS CALLOSUM
LEFT HEMISPHERE
PLASTICITY
Motor Development
The grasping reflex in newborns is an important hand skill, which by the fifth month progresses to reaching for and successfully grasping objects. This involves the coordination of multiple perceptual and motor abilities, and relates to the dynamic systems approach to development discussed in the first lesson.
VISUAL AND MOTOR SKILLSWALKINGMOBILITY FOSTERS INDEPENDENCEENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES
Knowledge Check
Question 1
Please select the two correct statements.
A one-year old should be able to walk, successfully grasp objects and have a healthy fear of heights.
The dynamic systems approach relates to child development in that increased environmental stimulation creates more synapses, and as neural communication becomes more complex, more systems work together, thus promoting development.
If damage occurs in one hemisphere of the brain, the other hemisphere can always take over.
The brain stops growing at the age of three, but the development of neural pathways and neural regeneration are life-long.
I don’t know
One attempt
Submit answer
You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.
Physical Growth
‹›
· Growth
As we saw in the first lesson when we discussed prenatal development, growth is cephalocaudal and proximal-distal. Growth is measured in height and weight. The most rapid growth occurs in the first year, where bodyweight triples from the time of birth. The body shape changes from being top-heavy to cylindrical as the center of mass moves to below the belly button, and dramatic growth spurts occur.
Height and weight are primarily determined by genetics, although environment also plays a significant role. Weight and height are also influenced by gender, where girls gain height and weight faster until about the age of fourteen. Variations also occur across nationality, ethnicity and socioeconomic class.
Weight
PREVALENCE OF OBESITY AMONG CHILDREN
CHILDHOOD OBESITY
OBESITY-RELATED HEALTH CONCERNS
ANOREXIA
BULIMIA NERVOSA
Knowledge Check
1
Question 1
Please select the two correct statements about the family’s role in healthy child development.
Parents should be aware of their children’s behavior as certain behaviors may be indicative of serious illnesses.
Absent fathers do not impact child well-being, and mothers are solely responsible for the emotional side of childrearing.
Parents should be aware of the pressure the following contradiction places on their children: the rate of obesity is increasing but society reveres slim, athletic body-types.
Genetics primarily determine height and weight. Thus, parents have little impact on their children’s health.
I don’t know
One attempt
Submit answer
You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.
Sexual Maturation
‹›
· Puberty
Puberty brings intense new drives, social pressures and new responsibilities. Pressure, stress and conflict often come along with these physical, emotional, psychological and social changes. Puberty begins when the pituitary gland is stimulated by the hypothalamus in the brain to release hormones. These hormones stimulate the adrenal cortex and gonads.
Knowledge Check
Question 1
Please select two environmental factors that influence how a child adjusts to adolescence.
Environmental changes
Social support
Menarche and spermarche
I don’t know
One attempt
Submit answer
You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.
Conclusion
In this lesson, we explored newborn, childhood and adolescent development. We started by looking at newborn’s reflexes, and how some of these reflexes are replaced by voluntary behaviors as development progresses. We then looked at infant states as innate forces as well as responses to the environment. Thereafter, we explored infant sensory and perceptual capacities, which included an investigation of auditory, taste, smell and touch development. We also explored early learning and memory, where we looked at how infants learn from classical and operant conditioning and imitation. The lesson then moved from newborns to children, from the perspectives of brain development, motor development and physical health. Lastly, we spoke about sexual maturation and the adjustments that adolescents are faced with.
KEY TERMS
References
Axia, G., Bonichini, S., & Benini, F. (1999). Attention and reaction to distress in infancy, a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 35, 500–504.
Banks, M. S., & Shannon, E. (1993). Spatial and chromatic visual efficiency in human neonates. In C. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 1–46). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Kramer, S. J. (1987). The perception of biomechanical motions. Implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 577–585.
Biringen, Z., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., & Appelbaum, M. I. (1995). Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dad: The role of upright locomotion and its timing. Child Development, 66, 499–514.
Blass, E., Ganchrow, J. R., & Steiner, J. E. (1984). Classical conditioning in newborn humans 2–48 hours of age. Infant Behaviorand Development, 7 , 223–234.
Blum, L. M. (2000). At the breast: Ideologies of breastfeeding and motherhood in the contemporary United States. Boston,MA: Beacon Press.
Butterfield, E. C., & Siperstein, G. N. (1972). Influence of contingent auditory stimulation upon non-nutritional suckle. In J. F. Bosma (Ed.), Third symposium on oral sensation and perception: The mouth of the infant (pp. 313–333). Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Campos, J. J., Langer, A., & Krowitz, A. (1970). Cardiac responses on the visual cliff in prelocomotor human infants. Science, 170, 196–197.
Canfield, R. L., & Haith, M. M. (1991). Young infants’ visual expectations for symmetrical and asymmetrical sequences. Developmental Psychology, 27, 198–208.
Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyschools/obesity/facts.htm
DeCasper, A., & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’ voices. Science, 12, 305–317.
Ellis, B. J., & Essex, M. J. (2007). Family environments, adrenarche, and sexual maturation: A longitudinal test of a life history model. Child Development, 78, 1799–1817.
Ge, X., Brody, G., Conger, R., Simons, R., & Murry, V. M. (2002). Contextual amplification of pubertal transition effects on deviant peer affiliation and externalizing behavior among African-American children. Developmental Psychology, 38, 42–54.
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr. (2001). Pubertal transition, stressful life events, and the emergence of gender differences in adolescent depressive symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 37, 404–417.
Gibson, E. J., & Walk, R. D. (1960). The “visual cliff.” Scientific American, 202, 64.
Gould, E., Reeves, A. J., Graziano, M. S., & Gross, C. G. (1999). Neurogenesis in the neocortex of adult primates. Science, 286, 548–555.
Herbert, J., Gross, J., & Hayne, H. (2006). Age-related changes in deferred imitations between 6 and 9 months of age. Infant Behaviorand Development, 29 , 136–139.
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. J. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in the human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167–178.
Ikonomov, O. G., Stoynev, A. G., & Shisheva, A. C. (1998). Integrative coordination of circadian mammalian diversity: Neuronal networks and peripheral clocks. Progress in Neurobiology, 54, 87–97.
Johnson, M. H. (1998). The neural basis of cognitive development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), & D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 1–49). New York: Wiley.
Johnson, M. H. (2005). Developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Johnson, S. P., Bremner, J. G., Slater, A., Mason, U., Foster, K., & Cheshire, A. (2003). Infants’ perception of object trajectories. Child Development, 74, 94–108.
Jones, M. C., & Bayley, N. (1950). Physical maturing among boys as related to behavior. Journal of Educational Psychology, 41, 129–148
Kellman, P. J., & Arterberry, M. E. (2006). Infant visual perception. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2, 6th ed., pp. 109–160). New York, NY: Wiley.
Kisilevsky, B. S., & Muir, D. W. (1991). Human fetal and subsequent newborn responses to sound and vibration. Infant Behaviorand Development, 14 , 1–26.
Kopp, C. B. (1994). Baby steps: The “whys” of your child’s behaviorin the first two years. New York, NY Freeman.
leCanuet, J.P., Fifer, W., Krasnegor, N., & Smotherman, W. (1995). Fetaldevelopment: A psychobiological perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 24, 470–476.
Mendle, J., Turkheimer, E., & Emery, R. E. (2007). Detrimental psychological outcomes associated with early pubertal timing in adolescent girls. Developmental Review, 27, 151–171.
Mennella, J. A., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1996). The human infants’ response to vanilla flavors in mother’s milk and formula. Infant Behaviorand Development, 19 , 13–19.
Morrongiello, B. A., Hewitt, K. L., & Gotowiec, A. (1991). Infant discrimination of relative distance in the auditory modality: Approaching versus receding sound sources. Infant BehaviorDevelopment, 14 , 187–208.
Nugent, J. K., Lester, B. M., & Brazelton, T. B. (1991). The cultural context of infancy, Vol. 2: Multicultural and interdisciplinary approaches to parent-infant relations. Westport, CT: Ablex.
Parke, R., & Gauvain, M. (2009).Child Psychology: A contemporary viewpoint (7th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Pascalis, O., De Schonen, S., Morton, J., Deruelle, C., & Fabre-Grenet, M. (1995). Mother’s face recognition by neonates: A replication and extension. Infant Behaviorand Development, 18 , 79–85.
Perry, B. D. (1997). Incubated in terror: Neurodevelopmental factors in the “cycle of violence.” In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Children in a violent society (pp. 124–149). New York: Guilford Press.
Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential facial response to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Development, 59, 1555–1568.
Rosenzweig, M. R., Leiman, A. S., & Breedlove, S. M. (1996). Biological psychology. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.
Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1999). The development of infant memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 80–85.
Rovee-Collier, C. K., & Shyi, G. (1992). A functional and cognitive analysis of infant long-term retention. In C. J. Brainard, M. L. Howe, & V. Reyna (Eds.), Development of long-term retention (pp. 3–55). New York: Springer-Verlag.
Saffran, J. R., Werker, J., & Werner, L. A. (2006). The infant’s auditory world. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Vol. Eds.), Cognition, perception & language (Vol. 2, 6th ed., pp. 58–108). New York, NY: Wiley.
Schaffer, H. R. (1971). The growth of sociability. London: Penguin.
Steinberg, L. (1987). Impact of puberty on family relations: Effects of pubertal status and pubertal timing. Developmental Psychology, 23, 451–460.
Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expression in response to taste and smell stimulation. In H. W. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 13, pp. 257–295). New York: Academic Press.
Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Bearman, S. K. (2001). Relation of early menarche to depression, eating disorders, substance abuse, and comorbid psychopathology among adolescent girls. Developmental Psychology, 37, 608–619.
Stiles, J. (2000). Spatial cognitive development following prenatal or perinatal focal brain injury. In H. S. Levin & J. Grafman (Eds.), Cerebral reorganization of function after brain damage (pp. 207–217). New York: Oxford University Press.
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (2006). Dynamic systems theory. In W. Damon & R. L. Lerner (Series Eds.), & R. L. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 258–312). New York: Wiley.
Tinsley, B. J. (2003). How children learn to be healthy. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Valsiner, J. (Ed.). (1989). Child development in cultural context. Toronto, Canada: Hogrefe and Huber.
Zeifman, D., Delaney, S., & Blass, E. M. (1996). Sweet taste, looking and calm in 2- and 4-week old infants: The eyes have it. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1090–1099.
Our essay writing company helps you enjoy campus life. We have committed and experienced tutors and academic writers who have a keen eye in writing original papers for university students. Buy high-quality essays online from our team of professional assignment writers. Every paper we deliver is original and crafted from scratch. Our expertise covers a wide range of assignments, regardless of their difficulty or academic level. From concise essays to extensive research papers, dissertations/theses, and coursework, we handle projects of all sizes.